By Léna Heuzé

False memories are phenomena of mnesic distortion that refer to the recall or recognition of events that never took place (Guyard & Piolino, 2006). The entire population seems to be subject to these memory errors. Thus, in order to better understand the formation of false memories in participants, three experimental paradigms were employed : the DRM paradigm (Deese, 1959 ; Roediger & McDermott, 1995), the misinformation paradigm (Loftus et al., 1978) and the predictive inference paradigm (Gras et al., 2008 ; Gras et al., 2011). We compared the production of these memory errors with respect to ageing, i.e. between young and old participants, while assuming that these older adults are more sensitive to the production of false memories as a function of advancing age.

To this end, 35 young and 64 older adults took part in our study. The participants began by performing the misinformation paradigm. They watched a short film and then answered a questionnaire containing misleading information. After a 45-minute delay, they performed a recognition task. They were then presented with the predictive inference paradigm. They were asked to read short sentences in small texts before performing a recognition task in which the predictive inferences that allowed them to create false memories were included. Finally, the test was concluded with the DRM paradigm. Participants were asked to read aloud lists of words before answering questions from the experimenter. The results partially validate our hypotheses since an age effect is found for two experimental paradigms, the DRM paradigm and the predictive inference paradigm, but not for the misinformation paradigm. Furthermore, correlations were found for the DRM and predictive inference paradigms, as well as for the DRM and misinformation paradigms for young adults.

The difference in the production of false memories between young and old participants can mainly be explained by a lack of monitoring for older adults. Numerous studies have shown that source control processes are less effective with advancing age (Gallo & Roediger, 2003 ; Dehon & Brédart, 2004). This prevents them from accessing distinct encoded information when retrieving information. Older adults prefer to rely on a sense of familiarity to retrieve information, which then allows them to produce false memories. Other difficulties were encountered for this population and may also explain the difference in false memory production between young and old participants. Indeed, it appears that difficulties in initiating and using effective strategies (Gallo, 2006) as well as a high level of semantic knowledge (McGillivray & Castel, 2017) are parameters that influence older people and allow them to produce more false memories.

Conversely, an equal rate of production of these memory errors between young and old participants for the misinformation paradigm can be explained by the recognition task. Indeed, studies have shown that a recognition task requires fewer cognitive processes, compared to a recall task (free or cueing) which requires many cognitive costs (Tulving, 1995 ; Froeger et al., 2014). But other explanations can be put forward such as the motivational degree of the task where the more participants judge the task as interesting and/or useful for them, the more they will be able to concentrate and thus produce fewer false memories (Auslander et al., 2017).

However, some limitations can be exposed such as the level of confidence adults have in their responses. Where numerous studies have shown that the more confident people, in this case older people, were in their responses, the more false memories they produced (Mueller et al., 2004 ; Dodson & Krueger, 2006). Thus, the objective of future studies will be to take this factor into account in order to observe a potential correlation between these two variables. 

In conclusion, this study shows that an age effect varies between experimental paradigms, being more pronounced for a DRM paradigm and a predictive inference paradigm, compared to a misinformation paradigm. It would therefore seem that the ecological nature of the task allows older adults to have correct monitoring processes that allow them to create fewer false memories than younger adults.

Auslander, M.-V., Thomas, A.-K., & Gutchess, A.-H. (2017). Confidence moderates the role of control beliefs in the context of age-related changes in misinformation susceptibility. Experimental Aging Research, 43(3), 305–322. http://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2017.1298960

Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall.  Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(1), 17-22.

Dehon, H., & Brédart, S. (2004). False memories: Young and older adults think of semantic associates at the same rate, but young adults are more successful at source monitoring. Psychology and Aging, 19(1), 191 197. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.191

Dodson, C. S., & Krueger, L. E. (2006). I misremember it well: Why older adults are unreliable eyewitnesses. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(5), 770-775. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193995

Froger, C., Toczé, C. & Taconnat, L., (2014). Comment la modification du comportement stratégique peut contribuer à l’explication du déclin mnésique au cours du vieillissement. L’Année psychologique, 2(2), 355-387.

https://doi.org/10.4074/S0003503314002061

Gallo, D. A., & Roediger, H. L. (2003). The effects of associations and aging on illusory recollection. Memory & Cognition, 31(7), 1036-1044.

https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196124

Gallo, D. (2006). Associative Illusions of Memory : False Memory Research in DRM and Related Tasks (Essays in Cognitive Psychology) (1re éd.). Psychology Press.

Gras, D., Tardieu, H., & Nicolas, S. (2008). Faux souvenirs et vieillissement : Les effets de l’âge sur les inférences prédictives. Psychologie et Neuropsychiatrie du Vieillissement, 6, 299-307. https://doi.org/10.1684/pnv.2008.0148

Gras, D., Tardieu, H., Piolino, P., & Nicolas, S. (2011). Presentation modality effect on false memories in younger and older adults: The use of an inference paradigm. Memory, 19(1),  92-102. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2010.537278

Loftus, E.-F., Miller, D.-G., & Burns, H.-J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(1), 19-31. http://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.4.1.19

McGillivray, S., & Castel, A. D. (2017). Older and Younger adult’s strategic control of metacognitive monitoring : the role of consequences, task experience and prior knowledge. Experimental Aging Research, 43(3), 233‑256. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073x.2017.1298956

Mueller-Johnson, K., & Ceci, S. J. (2004). Memory and suggestibility in older adults : live event participation and repeated interview. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(8), 1109‑1127. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1078

Piolino, P. (2006). La mémoire autobiographique : théorie et pratique en neuropsychologie. Revue Québécoise de Psychologie, 27(3), 1-20.

Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803-814. http://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803

Tulving, E. (1995). Organization of memory : Quo vadis ? The Cognitive Neurosciences, 839-847. Cambridge : The MIT Press.

Leave a Reply